UCSB Science Line
Sponge Spicules Nerve Cells Galaxy Abalone Shell Nickel Succinate X-ray Lens Lupine
UCSB Science Line
Home
How it Works
Ask a Question
Search Topics
Webcasts
Our Scientists
Science Links
Contact Information
I am doing a science fair project and my question is: "Do non-video game players have faster reactions than video game players? I was wondering if there is any way at all to improve your reaction time. If you have any information that you think will help me please answer to my question. Thank you!
Question Date: 2009-02-17
Answer 1:

I do not know the answer to your question although it seems reasonable to expect faster reaction times in video game players. Instead of me answering your question, let me propose that you measure reaction times as part of your science fair project. One idea you can use for this measurement is based on the free fall of objects and therefore adds some interesting physics application to your project.

Let us have first a little background in the physics of free-falling bodies. If an object starts from rest, then the vertical distance traveled is given by the formula:

d= (1/2) g t2

where g has a value of 32.2 ft/s2 or 9.81 m/s2. if using units from the International System instead of the British or Imperial System. t stands for the time of fall. For example, the distance an object falls (starting from rest) during the first 2 seconds is: 0.5x9.81x4 m = 19.62 m = 64.37 ft = 772 in. By doing some algebra, you can solve the above equation for t:

t = sqrt (2d/g), where sqrt means "take the square root of" (I encourage you to verify this by doing the algebra!)

And so, be able to find that, for example, the time it takes an object to fall 2m = 6.56 ft = 78.7 is t = 0.639s (rounded to 3 figures) which is quite a short time.

Human reaction times are even shorter: about 0.2 s. Of course this reaction time varies from one individual to the next, and also, in the same individual it varies from one day to the next or even from one hour to the next and this is why it would be very interesting to include the measurement of reaction times as part of the project.

Going back to our original equation, the distance an object falls (from rest) during 0.2s is, using 0.04 for the square of 0.2: 0.5x9.81x0.04 m = 0.1962 m = 19.62 cm = 7.72 in so it is a distance that can be measured my means of an ordinary school ruler.

One clever idea I found in a Physics textbook ("Physics" by Resnick and Halliday) many years ago is to use a ruler as both, the falling object and the length measuring device. You have the subject (whose reaction time you want to measure) place her forearm on a table with her hand just outside the edge of the table. She then keeps her thumb and index with a separation of around 0.5 cm while you hold the ruler vertically from the top at a height such that the lower end is at the same level as the upper part of the subjects thumb and index, just between the fingers but without touching them (see attached photos). Now, without prior notice (this is very important so you dont want to give yourself away), you release the ruler so that the subject has to first realize the ruler is now falling, and second, catch the ruler by closing her fingers without lowering the hand (that is why she needs to rest her forearm on top of a table). If, for example, you find that the upper part of her fingers holding the ruler is placed at a reading of 18.5 cm on the ruler, then by using our second equation above we find her reaction time to be (after converting 18.5 cm to 0.185 m):

t = sqrt(2x0.185/9.81) s = 0.194 s.

We can go one step farther and build a ruler (like the one shown in the photos) where we have readings directly in seconds (fractions of a second, actually) instead of having to read the length in cm and then, performing the calculation each time you perform a measurement. All we need is build a table, like the one below, that will allow us to draw the lines and numbers on a blank ruler (a strip of wood, plastic or aluminum about 30 or 40cm in length):
<
t (in seconds)length (in cm)
0.06 1.8
0.08____
0.10 ____
0.12 7.1
. . .. . .
0.20 19.6
0.22 ____
0.23 ____
0.24 28.3

I filled in some of the values on the column on the right of the table above so that you can build the rest of the table (fill in the blanks) using the first formula (for d). Look closely at the photos so you can have a better idea of what the ruler will look like.

Feel free to write if you are not sure of your results before constructing your "Reflexo-meter" ruler.

I really hope you include reaction times measurement as part of your project. Please let me know you did!

Picture_1
Picture_2


Answer 2:

The trouble you are going to find is having a control for your experiment. First, you need to find people who don't play video games. These people also need to do nothing else that would greatly improve their reflexes, like sports. Finding people who play neither video games nor sports is going to be challenging, since almost all young people do one or the other (or both!). There is also the problem that some video games use quick reflexes a whole lot more than others.

Reaction time can be improved by conditioning, and things like sports or other experiences can do it. I'm just concerned about how you will be able to get people for your sample.


Answer 3:

Sounds like a great project! I don't have any scientific studies about reaction time, but I do have two suggestions. First, as a good scientist, always ask yourself, "Is there any other explanation for what I'm seeing?" For example, you might test a bunch of people who play video games frequently and discover that they have very good reaction times. But does that mean the video games improved their reaction times or that they had very good reaction times already, so they were really good at video games and like to play them? Just because A and B go together doesn't mean A caused B. This is probably the biggest mistake in news stories in newspapers and the Internet.

So... you should do a "controlled study". That means you randomly select half of them to play video games (maybe draw names out of a hat), while the other half play cards or read. Measure the reaction times of both groups before and after the experiment. The group that doesn't play games will be your scientific "control group." Every good experiment should have a control group. That's what keeps you from fooling yourself with a seemingly good but wrong hypothesis (or question).

Second, I can offer you firsthand experience: when I started playing video games when I was young, my piano playing got better. And my piano playing improved my video gaming. So I'm one data point--but that's not really a scientific study. I wish I could join your study! :-)



Click Here to return to the search form.

University of California, Santa Barbara Materials Research Laboratory National Science Foundation
This program is co-sponsored by the National Science Foundation and UCSB School-University Partnerships
Copyright © 2020 The Regents of the University of California,
All Rights Reserved.
UCSB Terms of Use